microPublication

Get Your Data Out, Be Cited

  • About
    • Editorial Policies
      • Editorial Staff
      • Editorial Board
      • Criteria For Publication
      • Publishing Information
      • Data Sharing Policy
    • For Authors
      • Preparation And Submission Of A Manuscript
      • Peer Review Process
      • Following Acceptance
      • Appeals
    • For Reviewers
    • Why micropublish?
  • Submit a microPublication
  • Journals
    • microPublication Biology
      • Editorial Board
  • microPublications
    • Biology
      • Species
        • Arabidopsis
        • C. elegans
        • D. discoideum
        • Drosophila
        • Human
        • Mouse
        • S. cerevisiae
        • S. pombe
        • Xenopus
        • Zebrafish
      • Categories
        • Phenotype Data
        • Methods
        • Expression Data
        • Genotype Data
        • Integrations
        • Genetic Screens
        • Models of Human Disease
        • Software
        • Interaction data
        • Database Updates
        • Electrophysiology Data
        • Phylogenetic Data
        • Science and Society
        • Biochemistry
  • Contact
  • More
    • Archives
    • FAQs
    • Newsletter
microPublication / Biology / Variation between nematodes in a...
Variation between nematodes in a multi-sensory behavioral assay
Renae Ellis1 and Gareth Harris1
1Biology Program, 1 University Drive, California State University Channel Islands, Camarillo, Ca, 93012
Correspondence to: Gareth Harris (Gareth.harris@csuci.edu)
Figure 1. Variation between nematodes in a multi-sensory behavioral assay: Different nematode species show distinct food leaving rates during exposure to Repulsive cues: A-H A) Schematic of behavioral assay used to examine 2-nonanone-dependent food leaving (See methods section), B) Wild type N2 Caenorbabditis elegans vs Caenorhabditis nouraguensis examined for 2-nonanone-dependent food leaving, (n=5). Wild type N2 C. elegans (Black line), C. nouraguensis, (Red line), n.s=not statistically different, C) Wild type N2 Caenorhabditis elegans and Caenorhabditis portoensis were examined for 2-nonanone-dependent food leaving (n=6). Wild type N2 C. elegans, (Black line), C. portoensis, (Red line), D) Wild type N2 Caenorhabditis elegans vs Caenorhabditis remanei were examined for 2-nonanone-dependent food leaving (n=6). Wild type N2 C. elegans, (Black line), C. remanei, (Blue line), E-F) Caenorhabditis portoensis (slow leavers) and Caenorhabditis remanei (fast leavers) show same food leaving behavior in response to a second repellent, undiluted benzaldehyde (n=3 and, n=4, respectively). Wild type N2 C. elegans, (Black line), C. portoensis, (Red line), and wild type N2 C. elegans, (Black line), C. remanei, (Blue line), G) Caenorhabditis remanei shows fast food leaving behavior during exposure to 2-nonanone while residing on a Bacillus megaterium food patch (n=3). Wild type N2 C. elegans, (Black line), C. remanei, (Blue line), H) Caenorhabditis remanei shows normal sensitivity to 2-nonanone in the absence of food suggesting normal 2-nonanone repellent sensitivity (n=3). Y-axis=Mean avoidance Index (AI) vs X-axis=Time (minutes). Mean ± SEM, Student’s t-test, * p ≤ 0.05, ** p ≤ 0.01, *** p ≤ 0.001. For all assays, n=number of days tested, n.s=not statistically significant when comparing wild type N2 C. elegans to other Caenorhabditis strains tested in this study. All worms were examined using the same method when in the case of changing food types (Bacillus megaterium) or varying volatile chemicals used (2-nonanone (100%)/Benzaldehyde (100%). All Caenorhabditis species were examined in comparison to wild type N2 C. elegans as a control in identical behavioral assay conditions.

Description

An environment is often represented by numerous sensory cues. In order to better survive, an animal often needs to detect and process sensory cues simultaneously to make an appropriate behavioral decision. The Caenorhabditis elegans (C. elegans) genome encodes homologues of a significant number of molecules expressed in mammalian brains, which allows characterizing of the molecular and circuit basis for multi-sensory behavior during decision-making (Bargmann, 1998). In addition, studies have demonstrated various genes and neurons in behavioral differences previously observed in sensory behavior and decision-making when comparing different organisms, such as, nematodes. These variations in behavior involve differences in neuronal signals and neurons. For example, catecholamine signaling, neuropeptide Y-like receptors and pheromone chemoreceptors (Debono and Bargmann, 1998, Srinivasan et al.., 2008; Bendesky et al.., 2010, Mcgraph et al.., 2011). In this study, we investigate the behavioral differences across various species of nematodes, by examining multiple types of Caenorhabditis species and how they may differ in a multi-sensory behavioral assay (Harris et al.., 2019). The multisensory behavioral assay involves examining how different species of nematodes that are exposed to conflicting cues, behave when compared to the standard wild type, C. elegans Bristol N2 worms. We use a multi-sensory behavior paradigm to address these questions to determine any difference in ‘2-nonanone-dependent food leaving’, that assesses food leaving during exposure to the repellent 2-nonanone. Wild type N2 C. elegans were examined as the control in comparison to other Caenorhabditis species, including, Caenorhabditis remanei (C. remanei), Caenorhabditis nouraguensis (C. nouraguensis), and Caenorhabditis portoensis (C. portoensis).

Wild type N2 C. elegans in the lab are able to leave E. coli OP50 food lawns efficiently when exposed to undiluted 2-nonanone repellent (Harris et al.., 2019, Fig. 1A-B). In each experiment, we compared each nematode species to wild type C. elegans Bristol N2 (Caenorhabditis Genetics Center, CGC). A Caenorhabditis species, C. nouraguensis (Caenorabditis sp.17) left an E.coli OP50 food lawn during exposure to 2-nonanone similar to wild type N2 C. elegans, suggesting C. nouraguensis performs 2-nonanone-dependent food leaving with the same food leaving dynamics as wild type C. elegans N2 strain (Fig. 1B, Felix MA et al.., 2014). Interestingly, we then identified a species that left the food lawn significantly slower than wild type N2 C. elegans. The nematode, C. portoensis left the E. coli OP50 food lawn significantly slower during exposure to 2-nonanone (Fig. 1C). This nematode showed normal movement to the edge of the food lawn furthest away from the 2-nonanone, but remained at the edge of the food lawn for a significantly longer period without leaving the food (Fig. 1C, Felix MA, 2014). In addition to finding nematode species that show delayed food leaving compared to wild type N2 C. elegans, we also identified a nematode species that showed enhanced 2-nonanone-dependent food leaving. The nematode species, C. remanei, left E. coli OP50 food much faster during exposure to 2-nonanone (Fig. 1D, Sudhaus et al.,1974). We also confirmed that for the enhanced food leaving phenotype seen in C. remanei was not due to enhancement in sensitivity to 2-nonanone, using the standard 2-nonanone avoidance assay. We found that C. remanei showed no enhanced avoidance to 2-nonanone across a time course of 60 minutes (15, 30, 45, 60) (Troemel et al.., 1997, Fig. 1H). This suggests that nematodes, such as, C. remanei may exhibit differences in food leaving during repellent exposure due to potentially different response to food signals from the food lawn, during the decision to leave food and not due to differences in sensitivity to the 2-nonanone repellent that drives food leaving. It will be interesting to examine whether delayed food leaving observed in C. portoensis is related to differences in 2-nonanone sensitivity.

In addition, we have identified that the delayed and enhanced food leaving seen in C. portoensis and C. remanei, respectively, during exposure to 2-nonanone was also evident when exposing these two nematode species to a second odor repellent, undiluted benzaldehyde, that at high concentrations is also repulsive and drives food leaving in wild type N2 C. elegans (Fig. 1E and 1F, Harris et al.., 2019, Troemel et al., 1995; Luo et al.., 2008). Suggesting, the delayed and enhanced food leaving differences observed are general to multiple repellents. Finally, we examined changing food types during the assay to determine if we observed similar 2-nonanone-dependent food leaving behavior using different food patches. We show that using a Bacillus megaterium food patch instead of E. coli OP50 patch did not affect the enhanced food leaving in C. remanei, suggesting this faster food leaving behavior during exposure to 2-nonanone is general to multiple food types (Fig. 1G). Overall, we show an experiment that establishes variation between nematodes when measuring decision-making to leave a food patch during responses to conflicting cues, such as in this example, food and a repulsive cue, 2-nonanone. This provides an avenue to further examine differences in nematode behavior when exposing the worms to complex sets of attractive and repulsive cues.

Methods

Request a detailed protocol

All worms were cultivated under the standard conditions (Brenner et al.., 1974). Wild type (N2) hermaphrodites were used in this experiment. Wild type and other Caenorhabditis species were prepared in the presence of food (E. coli OP50) and staged to use young adult worms for all behavior assays.

Multi-sensory Behavioral Assay:

Wild type worms (N2) and all other strains in this study were assayed as previously described (Harris et al.., 2019). For all worms, E.coli OP50 was used as the food source. For preparation of assay plates, the NGM (Nematode Growth Medium) agar was made and poured into small assay plates (6 cm) and then allowed to cool and solidify (using standard NGM plate preparation protocol). After two days, a liquid suspension of E. coli OP50 was prepared; 40 mL of NGM media and E. coli OP50 colonies were added to the NGM media, this media was then placed at 26°C overnight. The next morning, the NGM media (E. coli OP50 culture) was centrifuged at 3500 rpm for approximately 15 minutes. 35 mL of the NGM was removed and discarded. The E. coli OP50 pellet was then re-suspended in 5 mL of NGM through mixing on a table top vortex and then 55 µL of this E. coli OP50 culture was then added to the center of an NGM agar assay plate (as seen in Fig. 1A, Schematic of behavioral assay). All worms were grown at 20-23°C on NGM plates that contained a lawn of E. coli OP50. For each assay, a population of young adult worms, 25-30 wild type (N2) and different nematodes were placed on separate NGM plates on the bacterial food source (E. coli, 55 µL). 2-nonanone (100%, 1µl) was then placed approximately two worm lengths from the food source. All nematodes examined were recorded every 5 minutes to determine how many worms have left the E. coli OP50 food lawn, until completion at 45 minutes. Food leaving during 2-nonanone exposure was defined as the number of worms that left the food lawn (not present on the food at each 5-minute time point across 45 minutes). For all benzaldehyde experiments, assays were performed in identical conditions to 2-nonanone-dependent food leaving assays. This shows the number of worms that have left the E. coli OP50 food lawn over 45 minutes. Assays with Bacillus megaterium food lawns used identical assay conditions and worm rearing, and wild type N2 C. elegans were used as a control for each condition tested. For data analysis, we analyzed the Mean ± SEM, Student’s t-test, * p ≤ 0.05, **p ≤ 0.01, ***p ≤ 0.001.

2-nonanone avoidance assay

To examine the avoidance of 2-nonanone, chemotaxis assays were performed essentially as previously described (Troemel et al.., 1997). Briefly, animals were placed in the center of a square plate that was divided into sectors A, B, C, D, E and F and 2 drops of 1 µl of undiluted 2-nonanone was added to one side and 2 drops of 1 µl ethanol was added to the opposite side of the plate as a control. Approximately 100 worms were used in each assay. 2-nonanone avoidance was analyzed by counting the number of worms in the sectors A-B, C-D, and E-F with E-F being furthest away from the 2-nonanone point sources. The avoidance index was calculated as the number of animals in sectors A and B (at 2-nonanone) minus the number of animals in the sectors E and F (at control) and normalized with the total number of animals in all 6 sectors on plate (Avoidance Index between 0 and -1.0). For all data analysis, a student’s t test was performed when comparing wild type N2 C. elegans hermaphrodite worms to any Caenorhabditis nematode species tested on the same day in parallel conditions. Mean ± SEM, Student’s t-test, * p ≤ 0.05, p ≤ 0.01**, p ≤ 0.001***

Reagents

Chemicals used in this study

All chemicals used in this study, including, 2-nonanone (CasRN: 821-55-6) and benzaldehyde (CasRN: 100-52-7) were purchased from Sigma Aldrich.

List of worm strains and bacterial strains used in these experiments

1) N2 Caenorhabditis elegans Bristol wild type worms were purchased from CGC

2) E.coli OP50 strain purchased from CGC

3) DA1880 Bacillus megaterium purchased from CGC

4) PB219 Caenorhabditis remanei was purchased from CGC

5) EG4788 Caenorhabditis portoensis was purchased from CGC

6) JU1825 Caenorhabditis nouraguensis was purchased from CGC

All strains were provided by the CGC(Caenorhabditis Genetics Center) at the University of Minnesota, which is funded by NIH Office of Research Infrastructure Programs (P40 OD010440).

Acknowledgments

We thank undergraduate lab members of the Harris lab for reading and helpful discussion about the data for relevant experiments. This project was planned and carried out by undergraduate researchers under supervision of the principal investigator of the lab(Dr. Gareth Harris). We thank CGC for strains for this project. We also thank our Biology program Instructional Support Technicians Catherine Hutchinson and Mike Mahoney for their diligent efforts with reagent ordering and helping with aspects of our lab.

References

Bargmann, C.I 1998. Neurobiology of the Caenorhabditis elegans Genome. Science Vol 282, Issue 5396, pp. 2028-2033.
10.1126/science.282.5396.202
Bendesky, A., Tsunozaki, M., Rockman, M.V., Kruglyak, L., Bargmann, C.I. 2011. Catecholamine receptor polymorphisms affect decision-making in C. elegans.
Nature 472(7343): 313-8.
PubMed
Brenner, S. 1974. The genetics of Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics 77(1): 71-94.
PubMed
de Bono, M., Bargmann, C.I. 1998. Natural Variation in a Neuropeptide Y Receptor Homolog Modifies Social Behavior and Food Response in C. elegans. Cell 94(5): 679-89.
PubMed
Félix, M.A., Braendle, C., Cutter, A.D. 2014. A streamlined system for species diagnosis in Caenorhabditis (Nematoda: Rhabditidae) with name designations for 15 distinct biological species, Public Library of Science. ONE 9. pp. 94723-94723.
PubMed
Harris, G., Linfield, G., Wu, T., Choi, M.K., Liu, H., and Zhang, Y. 2019. Molecular and Cellular modulators for multisensory integration in C. elegans. PloS Genetics 15(3): e1007706.
PubMed
Luo, L., Gabel, C.V., Ha, H.I., Zhang, Y., Samuel, A.D. 2008. Olfactory behavior of swimming C. elegans analyzed by measuring motile responses to temporal variations of odorants. J Neurophysiol 299(5): 2617-25.
PubMed
McGrath, P.T., Xu, Y., Ailion, M., Garrison, J.L., Butcher, R.A., Bargmann, C.I. 2011. Parallel evolution of domesticated Caenorhabditis species targets pheromone receptor genes. Nature 477(7364): 321-5.
PubMed
Srinivasan, J., Durak, O., Sternberg, P.W. 2008. Evolution of a polymodal sensory response network. BMC Biol. 6: 52.
PubMed
Sudhaus W. Zur Systematik, Verbreitung, Ökologie und Biologie neuer und wenig bekannter Rhabditiden (Nematoda). 2. Teil. Zoologische Jahrbücher (Systematik) 1974. 101: 417–465.
Troemel, E.R., Chou, J.H., Dwyer, N.D., Colbert, H.A., Bargmann, C.I. 1995. Divergent seven transmembrane receptors are candidate chemosensory receptors in C. elegans. Cell 83(2): 207-18.
PubMed
Troemel, E.R., Kimmel, B.E., Bargmann, C.I. 1997. Reprogramming chemotaxis responses: sensory neurons define olfactory preferences in C. elegans. Cell 91(2): 161-9.
PubMed

Funding

Laboratory start-up funds were used for this project(PI: Gareth Harris), Biology Program, CSUCI.

Author Contributions

Renae Ellis: Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing
Gareth Harris: Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing - review and editing, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Supervision, Conceptualization, Resources.

Reviewed By

Anonymous

History

Received: August 3, 2020
Revision received: November 13, 2020
Accepted: November 17, 2020
Published: November 28, 2020

Copyright

© 2020 by the authors. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International (CC BY 4.0) License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Citation

Ellis, R; Harris, G (2020). Variation between nematodes in a multi-sensory behavioral assay. microPublication Biology. 10.17912/micropub.biology.000330.
Download: RIS BibTeX
microPublication Biology is published by
1200 E. California Blvd. MC 1-43 Pasadena, CA 91125
The microPublication project is supported by
The National Institute of Health -- Grant #: 1U01LM012672-01
microPublication Biology:ISSN: 2578-9430